CREATIVITY NEWS

HOWARD GARDNER APPELS TO THE CRATIVE MIND:

What is Creativity or who are the Masters of Creativity?

"The acid test of creativity is whether after a person does his or her thing they actually change the way other people see the world," Howard Gardner said last Wednesday in King Concert Hall.
As an American developmental psychologist, researcher and published author Gardner launched Fredonia's 2010-2011 series of convocation programs entitled "Faces and Phases of Creativity."
In his lecture, Gardner attempted to reframe this concept of creativity through his research of well-known masters and creators. Among the seven primary masters of creativity he selected to study were: Albert Einstein, Pablo Picasso, T.S Eliot, Martha Graham, Sigmund Freud, Igor Stravinsky and Mohandas Gandhi. Gardner acknowledged that he chose these famous creators because they lived a century ago and have had a large seal of approval as creators and masters of their domain. Therefore each master of creativity he discussed represents a different domain of study and in return offered a novel way of seeing the world.
However, he adds that in order to really harness creativity to affect change, an individual must possess a certain temperament and discipline. "I don't think creativity is something you're born with, I think what you are born with are different talents but talents are not the same as being creative… being creative really has more to do with personality and temperament than how fast you master things."
Three key concepts, which, Gardner coined to describe this attitude towards creativity are: reflecting, leveraging and framing. He said that each of the masters he studied exhibited these practices.
Reflecting attributes to a creator's discipline to spending a lot of time thinking about what they are trying to achieve or the problem they are attempting to solve. This often requires a person to isolate him/herself from his/her social world for extended periods of time in order to remain focused on his/her goals. In order to do this, a creator needs a willingness to spend years on a problem, empowered by the passion to learn more.
Gardner says that in the case of these master creators, the inspiration really sparked the faculty for creativity when they discovered an anomaly. "When in any field, when you get into it, things erupt that don't make sense, you begin to draw things in a new way, or you hear sounds in a way that people haven't heard before, you ask a question like Einstein did, ‘what would happen if I were traveling at the speed of light?'"
Questions like these that went against the grain of conventional wisdom, Gardner said, naturally attributed to a creator's increased isolation.
Valerie Walender, project coordinator of the Dunkirk Historical Society future and a guest host of a future convocation event this semester regarding creativity said,
"I think it was interesting that he said a lot of these creative people were risk takers, because I think that is something I identified with as an artist." As a conceptual artist Walender began the "Free Community" initiative which is designed to build a community that is free of child abuse. By partnering local artists and creative individuals together with community organizations and businesses they brainstorm original ways of presenting the issue of child abuse to the community.
"This project here I created when I was 21 and I knew it was a good idea but not until probably thirty years later did I get a fellowship to make my idea into a reality, with many years refining that idea," Walender said.
Gardner's second principle of creativity, leveraging, consists of a person's ability to understand his or her own talents and use them to their advantage. According to Gardner's research, Sigmund Freud's intellectual strength came from his understanding of personal development and linguistics, yet his weaknesses existed in his spatial and musical understanding.
Additionally, Picasso's spatial, bodily and personal intellectual strengths are not without his weakness in understanding scholastics. Therefore he adds that each of these masters of creativity carries a sense of truthfulness about themselves and their work. They have seen things through with an uncluttered mind and a chilling directness to their goals that is unbridled by anything.
Finally, Gardner addressed framing as the most important principal he learned from the masters of creativity. Framing he said, "is what happens when things don't work," and the point in which the creator, "figures out what they can learn from and the lessons they can learn to make sure things go right next time." He added that framing requires a positive attitude and a willingness to persevere and frame failure into opportunity and change.
Gardner values these three concepts as lessons ordinary people can analyze and learn from when evaluating the life and work of historical and revolutionary creators.
Creativity for the Future
Gardner has extended his research and understanding of creativity to communities across America in order to facilitate change in the education system.
Project Zero is an educational research group at the Graduate School of Education at Harvard University that Gardner created with some of his colleagues. According to the Web site, http://www.pz.harvard.edu/ "Its mission is to understand and enhance learning, thinking and creativity in the arts, as well as humanistic and scientific disciplines, at the individual and institutional levels."
Gardner adds that Project Zero does not offer a set agenda for how to teach but rather works closely with educational leaders and organizing to strategize different methods of attaining creativity while teaching responsibility. One of his goals is to help educate young individuals to learn the importance of reflecting, leveraging and framing. Gardner expressed his concern for the educational system of America at the press conference before his lecture.
"This is a very unusual time in America; the country is very much divided on almost every issue. Interestingly it is not divided very much on education, particularly the pre-circular education. No child left behind is an issue that has more bi-partisan support than I know of."
The No Child Left Behind (NCLB) Act of 2001 supports a standards-based reform movement that calls for clear, measurable standards for all school students. Rather than norm-referenced rankings, a standards-based system measures each student against the concrete standard, instead of measuring how well the student performed compared to others.
The problem Gardner sees in standardizing educations is that too much depends upon testing, "What's been dominant is test scores, ranking and everything on those kind of quantitative measures." He mentioned that too much attention is given to the figures and not enough attention is allotted to the groundwork of education, to educate responsible and ethical people.
Gardner thinks that "Figures should entertain the bigger questions like, ‘what kinds of human beings do we want to have and what kind of society do we want to live in?'"
To be a great creator, he said a person should cultivate within themselves a respectful and ethical mind and this can begin at the earliest point in education.
Gardner is currently finishing his latest book, titled The Labor of Love: truth beauty and goodness reframed, educating for the virtues of the 21 century. He notes that this book will attempt to reason virtues in a post-modern society where new digital media continues to expose its unstable foundation daily.

http://www.fredonialeader.com/news/howard-gardner-appeals-to-the-creative-mind-1.1640823


The Creativity Crisis
For the first time, research shows that American creativity is declining. What went wrong—and how we can fix it.

Experts assess 10 drawings by adults and children for signs of out-of-the-box thinking. View gallery.
How Creative Are You?
Back in 1958, Ted Schwarzrock was an 8-year-old third grader when he became one of the “Torrance kids,” a group of nearly 400 Minneapolis children who completed a series of creativity tasks newly designed by professor E. Paul Torrance. Schwarzrock still vividly remembers the moment when a psychologist handed him a fire truck and asked, “How could you improve this toy to make it better and more fun to play with?” He recalls the psychologist being excited by his answers. In fact, the psychologist’s session notes indicate Schwarzrock rattled off 25 improvements, such as adding a removable ladder and springs to the wheels. That wasn’t the only time he impressed the scholars, who judged Schwarzrock to have “unusual visual perspective” and “an ability to synthesize diverse elements into meaningful products.”

The accepted definition of creativity is production of something original and useful, and that’s what’s reflected in the tests. There is never one right answer. To be creative requires divergent thinking (generating many unique ideas) and then convergent thinking (combining those ideas into the best result).

In the 50 years since Schwarzrock and the others took their tests, scholars—first led by Torrance, now his colleague, Garnet Millar—have been tracking the children, recording every patent earned, every business founded, every research paper published, and every grant awarded. They tallied the books, dances, radio shows, art exhibitions, software programs, advertising campaigns, hardware innovations, music compositions, public policies (written or implemented), leadership positions, invited lectures, and buildings designed.
Related Article: Forget Brainstorming »
Nobody would argue that Torrance’s tasks, which have become the gold standard in creativity assessment, measure creativity perfectly. What’s shocking is how incredibly well Torrance’s creativity index predicted those kids’ creative accomplishments as adults. Those who came up with more good ideas on Torrance’s tasks grew up to be entrepreneurs, inventors, college presidents, authors, doctors, diplomats, and software developers. Jonathan Plucker of Indiana University recently reanalyzed Torrance’s data. The correlation to lifetime creative accomplishment was more than three times stronger for childhood creativity than childhood IQ.

Like intelligence tests, Torrance’s test—a 90-minute series of discrete tasks, administered by a psychologist—has been taken by millions worldwide in 50 languages. Yet there is one crucial difference between IQ and CQ scores. With intelligence, there is a phenomenon called the Flynn effect—each generation, scores go up about 10 points. Enriched environments are making kids smarter. With creativity, a reverse trend has just been identified and is being reported for the first time here: American creativity scores are falling.

Kyung Hee Kim at the College of William & Mary discovered this in May, after analyzing almost 300,000 Torrance scores of children and adults. Kim found creativity scores had been steadily rising, just like IQ scores, until 1990. Since then, creativity scores have consistently inched downward. “It’s very clear, and the decrease is very significant,” Kim says. It is the scores of younger children in America—from kindergarten through sixth grade—for whom the decline is “most serious.”
The potential consequences are sweeping. The necessity of human ingenuity is undisputed. A recent IBM poll of 1,500 CEOs identified creativity as the No. 1 “leadership competency” of the future. Yet it’s not just about sustaining our nation’s economic growth. All around us are matters of national and international importance that are crying out for creative solutions, from saving the Gulf of Mexico to bringing peace to Afghanistan to delivering health care. Such solutions emerge from a healthy marketplace of ideas, sustained by a populace constantly contributing original ideas and receptive to the ideas of others.

It’s too early to determine conclusively why U.S. creativity scores are declining. One likely culprit is the number of hours kids now spend in front of the TV and playing videogames rather than engaging in creative activities. Another is the lack of creativity development in our schools. In effect, it’s left to the luck of the draw who becomes creative: there’s no concerted effort to nurture the creativity of all children.

Around the world, though, other countries are making creativity development a national priority. In 2008 British secondary-school curricula—from science to foreign language—was revamped to emphasize idea generation, and pilot programs have begun using Torrance’s test to assess their progress. The European Union designated 2009 as the European Year of Creativity and Innovation, holding conferences on the neuroscience of creativity, financing teacher training, and instituting problem-based learning programs—curricula driven by real-world inquiry—for both children and adults. In China there has been widespread education reform to extinguish the drill-and-kill teaching style. Instead, Chinese schools are also adopting a problem-based learning approach.

Plucker recently toured a number of such schools in Shanghai and Beijing. He was amazed by a boy who, for a class science project, rigged a tracking device for his moped with parts from a cell phone. When faculty of a major Chinese university asked Plucker to identify trends in American education, he described our focus on standardized curriculum, rote memorization, and nationalized testing. “After my answer was translated, they just started laughing out loud,” Plucker says. “They said, ‘You’re racing toward our old model. But we’re racing toward your model, as fast as we can.’ ”

Overwhelmed by curriculum standards, American teachers warn there’s no room in the day for a creativity class. Kids are fortunate if they get an art class once or twice a week. But to scientists, this is a non sequitur, borne out of what University of Georgia’s Mark Runco calls “art bias.” The age-old belief that the arts have a special claim to creativity is unfounded. When scholars gave creativity tasks to both engineering majors and music majors, their scores laid down on an identical spectrum, with the same high averages and standard deviations. Inside their brains, the same thing was happening—ideas were being generated and evaluated on the fly.

Researchers say creativity should be taken out of the art room and put into homeroom. The argument that we can’t teach creativity because kids already have too much to learn is a false trade-off. Creativity isn’t about freedom from concrete facts. Rather, fact-finding and deep research are vital stages in the creative process. Scholars argue that current curriculum standards can still be met, if taught in a different way.

To understand exactly what should be done requires first understanding the new story emerging from neuroscience. The lore of pop psychology is that creativity occurs on the right side of the brain. But we now know that if you tried to be creative using only the right side of your brain, it’d be like living with ideas perpetually at the tip of your tongue, just beyond reach.

When you try to solve a problem, you begin by concentrating on obvious facts and familiar solutions, to see if the answer lies there. This is a mostly left-brain stage of attack. If the answer doesn’t come, the right and left hemispheres of the brain activate together. Neural networks on the right side scan remote memories that could be vaguely relevant. A wide range of distant information that is normally tuned out becomes available to the left hemisphere, which searches for unseen patterns, alternative meanings, and high-level abstractions.

Having glimpsed such a connection, the left brain must quickly lock in on it before it escapes. The attention system must radically reverse gears, going from defocused attention to extremely focused attention. In a flash, the brain pulls together these disparate shreds of thought and binds them into a new single idea that enters consciousness. This is the “aha!” moment of insight, often followed by a spark of pleasure as the brain recognizes the novelty of what it’s come up with.

Now the brain must evaluate the idea it just generated. Is it worth pursuing? Creativity requires constant shifting, blender pulses of both divergent thinking and convergent thinking, to combine new information with old and forgotten ideas. Highly creative people are very good at marshaling their brains into bilateral mode, and the more creative they are, the more they dual-activate.

Is this learnable? Well, think of it like basketball. Being tall does help to be a pro basketball player, but the rest of us can still get quite good at the sport through practice. In the same way, there are certain innate features of the brain that make some people naturally prone to divergent thinking. But convergent thinking and focused attention are necessary, too, and those require different neural gifts. Crucially, rapidly shifting between these modes is a top-down function under your mental control. University of New Mexico neuroscientist Rex Jung has concluded that those who diligently practice creative activities learn to recruit their brains’ creative networks quicker and better. A lifetime of consistent habits gradually changes the neurological pattern.

A fine example of this emerged in January of this year, with release of a study by University of Western Ontario neuroscientist Daniel Ansari and Harvard’s Aaron Berkowitz, who studies music cognition. They put Dartmouth music majors and nonmusicians in an fMRI scanner, giving participants a one-handed fiber-optic keyboard to play melodies on. Sometimes melodies were rehearsed; other times they were creatively improvised. During improvisation, the highly trained music majors used their brains in a way the nonmusicians could not: they deactivated their right-temporoparietal junction. Normally, the r-TPJ reads incoming stimuli, sorting the stream for relevance. By turning that off, the musicians blocked out all distraction. They hit an extra gear of concentration, allowing them to work with the notes and create music spontaneously.

Charles Limb of Johns Hopkins has found a similar pattern with jazz musicians, and Austrian researchers observed it with professional dancers visualizing an improvised dance. Ansari and Berkowitz now believe the same is true for orators, comedians, and athletes improvising in games.

The good news is that creativity training that aligns with the new science works surprisingly well. The University of Oklahoma, the University of Georgia, and Taiwan’s National Chengchi University each independently conducted a large-scale analysis of such programs. All three teams of scholars concluded that creativity training can have a strong effect. “Creativity can be taught,” says James C. Kaufman, professor at California State University, San Bernardino.

What’s common about successful programs is they alternate maximum divergent thinking with bouts of intense convergent thinking, through several stages. Real improvement doesn’t happen in a weekend workshop. But when applied to the everyday process of work or school, brain function improves.

So what does this mean for America’s standards-obsessed schools? The key is in how kids work through the vast catalog of information. Consider the National Inventors Hall of Fame School, a new public middle school in Akron, Ohio. Mindful of Ohio’s curriculum requirements, the school’s teachers came up with a project for the fifth graders: figure out how to reduce the noise in the library. Its windows faced a public space and, even when closed, let through too much noise. The students had four weeks to design proposals.

Working in small teams, the fifth graders first engaged in what creativity theorist Donald Treffinger describes as fact-finding. How does sound travel through materials? What materials reduce noise the most? Then, problem-finding—anticipating all potential pitfalls so their designs are more likely to work. Next, idea-finding: generate as many ideas as possible. Drapes, plants, or large kites hung from the ceiling would all baffle sound. Or, instead of reducing the sound, maybe mask it by playing the sound of a gentle waterfall? A proposal for double-paned glass evolved into an idea to fill the space between panes with water. Next, solution-finding: which ideas were the most effective, cheapest, and aesthetically pleasing? Fiberglass absorbed sound the best but wouldn’t be safe. Would an aquarium with fish be easier than water-filled panes?

Then teams developed a plan of action. They built scale models and chose fabric samples. They realized they’d need to persuade a janitor to care for the plants and fish during vacation. Teams persuaded others to support them—sometimes so well, teams decided to combine projects. Finally, they presented designs to teachers, parents, and Jim West, inventor of the electric microphone.

Along the way, kids demonstrated the very definition of creativity: alternating between divergent and convergent thinking, they arrived at original and useful ideas. And they’d unwittingly mastered Ohio’s required fifth-grade curriculum—from understanding sound waves to per-unit cost calculations to the art of persuasive writing. “You never see our kids saying, ‘I’ll never use this so I don’t need to learn it,’ ” says school administrator Maryann Wolowiec. “Instead, kids ask, ‘Do we have to leave school now?’ ” Two weeks ago, when the school received its results on the state’s achievement test, principal Traci Buckner was moved to tears. The raw scores indicate that, in its first year, the school has already become one of the top three schools in Akron, despite having open enrollment by lottery and 42 percent of its students living in poverty.

With as much as three fourths of each day spent in project-based learning, principal Buckner and her team actually work through required curricula, carefully figuring out how kids can learn it through the steps of Treffinger’s Creative Problem-Solving method and other creativity pedagogies. “The creative problem-solving program has the highest success in increasing children’s creativity,” observed William & Mary’s Kim.

The home-game version of this means no longer encouraging kids to spring straight ahead to the right answer. When UGA’s Runco was driving through California one day with his family, his son asked why Sacramento was the state’s capital—why not San Francisco or Los Angeles? Runco turned the question back on him, encouraging him to come up with as many explanations as he could think of.

Preschool children, on average, ask their parents about 100 questions a day. Why, why, why—sometimes parents just wish it’d stop. Tragically, it does stop. By middle school they’ve pretty much stopped asking. It’s no coincidence that this same time is when student motivation and engagement plummet. They didn’t stop asking questions because they lost interest: it’s the other way around. They lost interest because they stopped asking questions.

Having studied the childhoods of highly creative people for decades, Claremont Graduate University’s Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi and University of Northern Iowa’s Gary G. Gute found highly creative adults tended to grow up in families embodying opposites. Parents encouraged uniqueness, yet provided stability. They were highly responsive to kids’ needs, yet challenged kids to develop skills. This resulted in a sort of adaptability: in times of anxiousness, clear rules could reduce chaos—yet when kids were bored, they could seek change, too. In the space between anxiety and boredom was where creativity flourished.

It’s also true that highly creative adults frequently grew up with hardship. Hardship by itself doesn’t lead to creativity, but it does force kids to become more flexible—and flexibility helps with creativity.

In early childhood, distinct types of free play are associated with high creativity. Preschoolers who spend more time in role-play (acting out characters) have higher measures of creativity: voicing someone else’s point of view helps develop their ability to analyze situations from different perspectives. When playing alone, highly creative first graders may act out strong negative emotions: they’ll be angry, hostile, anguished. The hypothesis is that play is a safe harbor to work through forbidden thoughts and emotions.

In middle childhood, kids sometimes create paracosms—fantasies of entire alternative worlds. Kids revisit their paracosms repeatedly, sometimes for months, and even create languages spoken there. This type of play peaks at age 9 or 10, and it’s a very strong sign of future creativity. A Michigan State University study of MacArthur “genius award” winners found a remarkably high rate of paracosm creation in their childhoods.

From fourth grade on, creativity no longer occurs in a vacuum; researching and studying become an integral part of coming up with useful solutions. But this transition isn’t easy. As school stuffs more complex information into their heads, kids get overloaded, and creativity suffers. When creative children have a supportive teacher—someone tolerant of unconventional answers, occasional disruptions, or detours of curiosity—they tend to excel. When they don’t, they tend to underperform and drop out of high school or don’t finish college at high rates.

They’re quitting because they’re discouraged and bored, not because they’re dark, depressed, anxious, or neurotic. It’s a myth that creative people have these traits. (Those traits actually shut down creativity; they make people less open to experience and less interested in novelty.) Rather, creative people, for the most part, exhibit active moods and positive affect. They’re not particularly happy—contentment is a kind of complacency creative people rarely have. But they’re engaged, motivated, and open to the world.

The new view is that creativity is part of normal brain function. Some scholars go further, arguing that lack of creativity—not having loads of it—is the real risk factor. In his research, Runco asks college students, “Think of all the things that could interfere with graduating from college.” Then he instructs them to pick one of those items and to come up with as many solutions for that problem as possible. This is a classic divergent-convergent creativity challenge. A subset of respondents, like the proverbial Murphy, quickly list every imaginable way things can go wrong. But they demonstrate a complete lack of flexibility in finding creative solutions. It’s this inability to conceive of alternative approaches that leads to despair. Runco’s two questions predict suicide ideation—even when controlling for preexisting levels of depression and anxiety.

In Runco’s subsequent research, those who do better in both problem-finding and problem-solving have better relationships. They are more able to handle stress and overcome the bumps life throws in their way. A similar study of 1,500 middle schoolers found that those high in creative self-efficacy had more confidence about their future and ability to succeed. They were sure that their ability to come up with alternatives would aid them, no matter what problems would arise.

When he was 30 years old, Ted Schwarzrock was looking for an alternative. He was hardly on track to becoming the prototype of Torrance’s longitudinal study. He wasn’t artistic when young, and his family didn’t recognize his creativity or nurture it. The son of a dentist and a speech pathologist, he had been pushed into medical school, where he felt stifled and commonly had run-ins with professors and bosses. But eventually, he found a way to combine his creativity and medical expertise: inventing new medical technologies.

Today, Schwarzrock is independently wealthy—he founded and sold three medical-products companies and was a partner in three more. His innovations in health care have been wide ranging, from a portable respiratory oxygen device to skin-absorbing anti-inflammatories to insights into how bacteria become antibiotic-resistant. His latest project could bring down the cost of spine-surgery implants 50 percent. “As a child, I never had an identity as a ‘creative person,’ ” Schwarzrock recalls. “But now that I know, it helps explain a lot of what I felt and went through.”

Creativity has always been prized in American society, but it’s never really been understood. While our creativity scores decline unchecked, the current national strategy for creativity consists of little more than praying for a Greek muse to drop by our houses. The problems we face now, and in the future, simply demand that we do more than just hope for inspiration to strike. Fortunately, the science can help: we know the steps to lead that elusive muse right to our doors.
http://www.newsweek.com/2010/07/10/the-creativity-crisis.html


Giving birth to creativity
ELEANOR FITZSIMONS
Recent research suggests that pregnancy boosts women’s creative output
PREGNANCY, NOT to mention its eventual consequences, changes us. Hormones flood our bodies and we are forced to slow down and refocus. Much has been written about the effects on a woman’s behaviour; from welling up at the story of a lost cat being reunited with its owner to retching at the smell of raw meat – that particular symptom turned my hapless family and I vegetarian for many months. However, one rarely discussed consequence of pregnancy is its impact on a woman’s creativity. Surely an event that leads to raw emotions and fundamental changes in our thought processes will inevitably have some impact on our creative output?
Recently scientists have tried to quantify the effects of pregnancy and to measure and document behavioural changes long supported by anecdotal evidence. A team of neuroscientists led by Craig Kinsley, associate professor of psychology at the University of Richmond in Virginia, found that the specific influx of hormones during pregnancy and labour effectively rewire a woman’s brain and fundamentally change the manner in which she interacts with the world.
The team also found that this transformation endures for far longer than nine months. Hormones, such as oestrogen and oxytocin released during pregnancy and nursing, dramatically enrich parts of the brain linked to memory and learning. The results of their study, entitled Motherhood Enhances Learning and Memory: Accompanying Alterations in Neuronal and Glial Morphology , were presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience. The research, conducted on pregnant and recently pregnant rats, found they were braver, more energetic and more inquisitive than previously. They completed complex mazes with fewer mistakes and retained knowledge for longer.
So, what of humans? Experiences seem to differ depending on who you talk to. Some women view pregnancy as a hugely creative time while others discern little change. Novelist and poet Evelyn Cosgrave speaks of her first pregnancy as a “very introspective time”. She wrote poetry, much of it focused on the daughter she carried, but struggled to truly get to grips with her emotions. During her second pregnancy she started to write novels and found that the limited time available helped her to focus and let less important things slide. “I typed with one hand and fed with the other,” she says.
Fellow novelist Amanda Hearty describes how the characters for her first novel, Are You Ready , “just came to me” during the early stages of pregnancy. She completed it in four months and had written 50 pages of the next one before daughter Holly put in an appearance. While pregnant she also completed a script-writing course and designed a board game.
Artist Margaret Corcoran painted her stunning Golden Fleece award-winning series of paintings, An Enquiry , while pregnant with her second daughter. The paintings follow older daughter Georgia’s investigation of the Milltown Rooms in the National Gallery. Corcoran says, “They are really special to me and I don’t have any because they all sold. Georgia was 12 or 13 at the time, at that transitional stage.”
So, how did pregnancy affect her work? “I sat at a table resting my feet on my tool box. I had Lyric FM playing as I worked. I was quite big but experienced this very peaceful, centred, contented feeling. I had to work at pregnancy pace. I couldn’t sleep so I was up at six in the morning, greeting the day while everyone else slept.” The paintings are hailed as reminiscent of early Degas in style and composition.
Corcoran says she was “definitely more intuitive when pregnant. These paintings are all about children, puberty and new birth. The green background – from a Degas – represents youth. I painted from photographs so I was painting every hair on my daughter’s head. I was incredibly intimate with one daughter and heavily pregnant with the other at this transient time in life.”
Pregnancy-related creativity is in abundant evidence elsewhere. Helen Dunmore wrote her Orange Prize-winning third novel, A Spell of Winter , while pregnant. Novelist Barbara Kingsolver suffered from insomnia and is certain the condition helped her write her debut novel, The Bean Trees. Jodie Picoult wrote her first novel,Songs of the Humpback Whale , while pregnant with her first child.
Each individual pregnancy seems to affect the ability of an expectant mother to explore her creativity. Writer Rebecca Brown says she was “too ill, tired and preoccupied. Now though four weeks after giving birth I have lots of story ideas and plans.” The irrepressible and hugely creative cartoonist Annie West is adamant that she “didn’t do a thing for about six years. Then that wonderful thing called school appeared. Saved my career.”
It seems the rats were right. Although often delayed by a preoccupation with morning sickness and demanding newborns, the creative benefits of pregnancy linger on and influence output in ways that benefit the expectant mother and her audience.
http://www.irishtimes.com/